Results
of the Knox College Creepiness
Study
First of all, Sara Koehnke
and myself (Frank McAndrew) would like to thank all of you who took the
time to complete our Creepiness Survey about a year ago.
Sorry
that it took so long to get the results out to you!
We have
just submitted a paper to a peer-reviewed psychology journal and hope
to get it published. Please do not repost this anywhere on the
intenet, and if you are a journailist or science writer, please do not
write about any of this without contacting me first.
It would be too boring and tedious to
give you every scrap of what we found, but I will try to provide a
summary of the high spots without all of the statistical mumbo-jumbo.
If anyone want more details, feel free to email me at :
fmcandre@knox.edu.
Here are some of the details:
WHO WAS IN THE STUDY? (besides you):
Participants were recruited
through invitations to Facebook events that were created by the
researchers, through campus-wide emails distributed to students,
faculty, and staff at a liberal arts college in the American Midwest,
and through the “Social Psychology Network”
website. Volunteers were encouraged to forward the link to the online
survey to their friends and acquaintances. A brief description of the
study and a link to the survey were posted on the invitation
page. This
resulted in a final sample of 1,341 individuals (1,029 females, 312
males) ranging in age from 18 to 77 with a mean age of 28.97 (SD =
11.34). We did not ask participants to report their country
of
origin, but in an unrelated study using an identical recruitment
strategy, respondents from 54 different nations were
acquired.
Thus, although our sample was primarily American, we are confident that
there was significant international representation.
Participants
had to check a box confirming that they were at least 18 years of age
before they could access the survey.
A GENERAL SUMMARY OF THE HIGH POINTS:
Given
how frequently the concept of
“creepiness” is invoked in everyday life, it is
surprising
that it has not been studied by psychologists. This study attempted to
uncover the cues that are used to label someone as
“Creepy”
and to identify the basic elements of creepiness. An
international sample of 1,341 individuals responded to an online survey
about creepiness. The results revealed that males are
perceived
as creepier than females and that females are more likely to associate
sexual threat with creepiness. Behaviors and characteristics associated
with unpredictability are also predictors of creepiness, as are some
occupations and hobbies. The results are consistent with the
hypothesis that being “creeped out” is an adaptive
emotional response to uncertainty about the presence of threat that
enables us to maintain vigilance during times of uncertainty.
THE
DETAILS:
Tests of
Hypotheses.
The first prediction was that creepy individuals would be expected to
be males more often than females. This prediction was
assessed
directly via the question that asked people to choose whether a creepy
person was more likely to be a male or a female. 95.3% of our
respondents thought that creepy people were much more likely to be
males than females, a finding that was highly significant.
This
perception was equally likely to be held by male participants (95.5%
vs. 4.5%) and female participants (95.2% vs. 4.8%). Thus, our
first prediction was supported: males are creepier than females.
The second prediction was that females are more likely to perceive a
sexual threat from a creepy person than are males. This
hypothesis was tested by comparing male and female
responses to two items: The degree to which steering a conversation
toward sex was perceived as a probable characteristic of a creepy
person and the degree to which the respondent agreed with the statement
that the creepy person “has a sexual interest in
me.”
The prediction was supported by both of these items. Females
were
more likely than males to think that steering a conversation toward sex
was characteristic of a creepy person, and they were also more likely
to think that the creepy person had a sexual interest in them.
The third prediction was that occupations would differ in their level
of creepiness according to how threatening or strange the
“subject matter” of the occupation is.
The means and
standard deviations of the creepiness ratings for the 21 stimulus
occupations are displayed in the Table below. The analysis revealed that the differences in how occupations were
rated
was highly significant; all of the occupations except two (construction
workers and computer software engineers) were significantly different
from each other. However, only four occupations were
judged
to be significantly higher than the neutral value of
“3” on
the creepiness rating scale: Clowns, Taxidermists, Sex Shop Owners, and
Funeral Directors. Therefore, it appears that occupations
associated with death (taxidermy and funeral directors) or reflective
of a fascination with sex (sex shop owners) are perceived as creepy;
clowns were the creepiest of all.
Table 1
Creepiness Ratings of
Occupations
Occupation
Mean
SD
Clown
3.71
1.24
Taxidermist
3.69
1.19
Sex Shop Owner
3.32
1.30
Funeral Director
3.22
1.23
Taxi Driver
2.86
1.19
Unemployed
2.83
1.29
Clergy
2.57
1.28
Janitor
2.51
1.19
Garbage Collector
2.25
1.12
Guard
2.18
1.08
Writer
2.14
1.08
Actor
2.13
1.02
Construction Worker*
2.09
1.09
Computer Software
Engineer* 2.09
1.11
Cafeteria Worker
2.08
1.06
Financial Adviser
1.78
0.98
Doctor/Physician
1.77
0.96
College Professor
1.67
0.86
Farmer
1.65
0.90
Teacher
1.57
0.82
Meteorologist
1.53
0.83
Note: Occupations marked with an asterisk are not significantly
different from each other (Tukey HSD = .01). Ratings were
made on
a “1” (not very creepy) to “5”
(Very creepy)
scale.
The fourth prediction was that things that make a person unpredictable
also predict creepiness. One item among the ratings of creepy
individuals (“I am uncomfortable because I cannot predict how
he
or she will behave”) and one item among the items assessing
beliefs about creepy people (“Even though someone may seem
creepy, I usually think that I understand his or her
intentions”)
allowed a direct test of this prediction. Collectively, the
results of the analyses of these two items indicate that
unpredictability is indeed an important component of creepy behavior.
Exploratory Analyses:
The many items in our survey afford ample opportunities for exploration
of the elements of creepiness. Our first step in this
direction
was to combine items that seemed to be measuring the same thing within
the two longest sections of our questionnaire. The first
section
contained 44 items assessing the likelihood that a creepy person
described by one’s trusted friend would display a particular
behavior or possess a particular physical characteristic. In
an
attempt to reduce the number of variables to be
analyzed, these 44 items were subjected to statistical manipulation called "factor
analysis." The factor analysis was able to identify only one
factor
that connected multiple variables. This factor included 15 of
the
44 items, all of which reflected a nonverbal behavior or physical
characteristic of creepy people. A new variable called
Appearance/NVB was calculated by computing a mean based upon the scores
of each individual on these 15 items. The 15 items that
comprised
this new variable are as follows.
The person stood too close
The person had greasy hair
The person had a peculiar smile
The person had bulging eyes
The person had long fingers
The person had unkempt hair
The person had very pale skin
The person had bags under his or her
eyes
The person was dressed oddly
The person licked his or her lips
frequently
The person was wearing dirty
clothes
The person laughed at unpredictable
times
The person made it nearly impossible to leave the conversation
without appearing rude
The person relentlessly steered the conversation toward one topic
This new composite Appearance/NVB variable along with the remaining 29
items from the first portion of the questionnaire were analyzed via
one-sample t tests to determine which of these characteristics was
significantly above the neutral point of “3,” and
therefore
very likely to be a characteristic of a creepy person. The
following elements were thought to be very likely to be found in a
creepy person:
The appearance and nonverbal behavior items in the composite variable
(Appearance/NVB), being of the opposite sex (probably due to the
predominantly female sample in our study), being extremely thin, not
looking the interaction partner in the eye, asking to take a picture of
the interaction partner, watching people before interacting with them,
asking about details of one’s personal life, having a mental
illness, talking about his/her own personal life, displaying
too
much or too little emotion, being older, and steering the conversation
toward sex.
Similarly, the section of the questionnaire consisting of 15 items that
reflected beliefs about the nature of creepy people was subjected to
a factor analysis.
The analysis yielded four factors on which at least two items
loaded.
The first factor tapped into how fearful or anxious the person felt
while interacting with a creepy person, and it included the following
items; Each statement began with the expression
“When I
meet someone that seems creepy . . .
I am sure that the person intends to harm me
I am uncomfortable because I cannot predict how he or she will behave
I feel anxious
I believe that he or she is intentionally hiding something from
me
The second factor reflected how intimately involved one would be with a
creepy person, and it consisted of two items:
People are less creepy if I know I won’t have to speak to
them ever again
People are creepier when I meet them online compared to
face-to-face
The third factor measured the extent to which creepiness is an inherent
part of the individual, and it consisted of two
items:
Some people can do the exact same behavior as someone else and one
person can be perceived as creepy while the other person is
not
Behaviors often admired in “bad guys” in movies and
TV shows are actually really creepy if done in real life
The fourth factor reflected the extent to which people willfully
deviate from social norms, and it consisted of two items:
When I meet someone who seems creepy, I expect him or her to follow the
usual rules for socially acceptable behavior
People choose to act in a creepy manner
New composite variables labeled “fearfulness,”
“proximity,” “individual
creepiness,” and
“non-normativity,” were calculated by computing a
mean of
the items that loaded on each factor. These four composite variables
along with the remaining 5 items from the last portion of the
questionnaire were analyzed to determine which of these characteristics
was significantly different from neutral point of
“3,” and
therefore strongly believed to be characteristics of a creepy person.
The following things were believed to be true of a creepy
person:
They make us fear fearful/anxious (composite fearfulness variable)
Creepiness resides in the individual more than in his/her behavior
(composite individual creepiness variable)
We think they may have a sexual interest in us
They are creepy when they exhibit multiple
“symptoms” of creepiness rather than just one
The expected intimacy and frequency of interaction with the person
moderates perceptions of creepiness
Creepy people are unable to change, but they do not necessarily have
bad intentions
People who follow social rules of behavior are not perceived as creepy
There was also one final item in which participants chose among
“yes,” “no,” and
“unsure” in response to the question “Do
most creepy
people know that they are creepy?” The
responses were
115 “yes” (8.6%), 797 “no”
(59.4%), and 429
“unsure” (32%), indicating that our participants
did not
believe that most creepy people know that they are creepy.
There were many significant correlations between the age of the
participant and his/her responses to the items in the survey. The
general finding of interest was that older people seemed to be less
alarmed by creepy people than are younger people, being less likely to
perceive sexual threat or intended harm. They also expressed
less
anxiety at the prospect of interacting with a creepy person.
Just for fun, we asked our participants to list two hobbies that they
thought of as creepy. Easily, the most frequently mentioned
creepy hobbies involved collecting things (listed by 341 of our
participants). Collecting dolls, insects, reptiles, or body
parts
such as teeth, bones, or fingernails was considered especially
creepy. The second most frequently mentioned creepy hobby
(listed
by 108 participants) involved some variation of
“watching.” Watching, following, or
taking pictures
of people (especially children) was thought to be creepy by many of our
participants, and bird watchers were considered creepy by many as
well. A fascination with pornography or exotic sexual
activity
and taxidermy were also frequently mentioned.